Pigeons aren’t so bird-brained after all, they discriminate space and time
University of IowaIn experiments, pigeons were shown a static horizontal line and had to judge its length or the amount of time it was visible to them. Pigeons judged longer lines to also have longer duration and judged lines longer in duration to also be longer in length.
What that means, says Edward Wasserman, Stuit Professor at the UI, is pigeons use a common area of the brain to judge space and time, suggesting that these abstract concepts are not processed separately. Similar results have been found with humans and other primates.
The finding adds to growing recognition in the scientific community that lower-order animal species - such as birds, reptiles, and fish - are capable of high-level, abstract decision-making.
Not a bird brain
“Indeed, the cognitive prowess of birds is now deemed to be ever closer to that of both human and nonhuman primates,” says Wasserman, who has studied intelligence in pigeons, crows, baboons, and other animals for more than four decades. “Those avian nervous systems are capable of far greater achievements than the pejorative term ‘bird brain’ would suggest.”
Humans are able to perceive space and time, even without the aid of inventions such as a watch or a ruler. The region of the brain that helps humans make those abstract concepts more tangible is the parietal cortex, part of the cerebral cortex and the outermost layer of the brain. The cerebral cortex is known to be a locus of higher thought processes, including speech and decision-making, and the four lobes that comprise it, including the parietal cortex, process different types of sensory information. But the pigeon brain doesn’t have a parietal cortex, or at least one developed enough to be distinct. So, the birds must employ another area of the brain to discriminate between space and time - or perhaps there’s a common evolutionary mechanism in the central nervous system shared by early primates and birds. Wasserman and his team wanted to find out.
An interplay of space and time
They put pigeons through a series of tasks called the 'common magnitude' test. Put simply, the birds were shown on a computer screen a horizontal line either 6 or 24 centimeters long for either 2 or 8 seconds. If they correctly reported (by pecking one of four visual symbols) the length or the duration of the line, they received food. The test then became more nuanced. The researchers introduced additional line lengths, thus adding greater variability in judging whether a line was short or long; they also presented the line to the pigeons for either a shorter or longer duration. “The task now forces pigeons to process time and space simultaneously because they cannot know on which dimension they’re going to be tested,” Wasserman says.
The researchers found that the length of the line affected the pigeons’ discrimination of line duration, and the duration of the line affected the pigeons’ discrimination of line length. This interplay of space and time paralleled research done with humans and monkeys and revealed the common neural coding of these two physical dimensions. Researchers previously believed that the parietal cortex was the locus of this interplay. However, because pigeons lack an apparent parietal cortex, Wasserman’s findings suggest this isn’t always the case.
Benjamin De Corte, a third-year graduate student with the UI’s Iowa Neuroscience Institute and the Department of Neurology who helped design and execute the experiments, says the results show pigeons process space and time in ways similar to humans and other primates. “The cortex is not unique to judging space and time,” says De Corte, who is first author on the paper. “The pigeons have other brain systems that allow them to perceive these dimensions.”
The findings of this study are published in the scientific journal Current Biology.
Text: University of Iowa
Photos: Pixabay; Kathryn Gamble